Tuesday, 12 August 2014

Synthetic phonics and irregular word reading: Cause for concern?


There have been numerous concerns about the introduction of a phonics focused synthetic phonics approach to the teaching of reading in England.  One of the concerns often highlighted is that a phonics focused approach does not suit the opaque (i.e., irregular) nature of the English writing system.  For example, words such as ‘aisle’, ‘pint’, ‘yacht’ are all irregular and pose difficulties for children learning to read.  Indeed, of all the alphabetic writing systems, English is one of the most irregular and research has shown that the ease with which a child learns to read is related to the orthographic depth of the writing system (Ellis et al, 2004).  Therefore learning to read English is challenging.

However, the argument that a phonics focused synthetic phonics approach is unsuitable to teach children to read English is an argument from which I have seen no research evidence.  Examples have been given of children being unable to read ‘high frequency’ words such as ‘the’ – sounding out ‘t’’h’’e’ then blending them together and forming an inaccurate pronunciation.  However, to me, this is better than I’ve seen prior to the introduction of synthetic phonics, where the first three words of the reading test that I used to administer (British Ability Scales II) were ‘the’ ‘up’ and ‘on’, and read by a number of students as ‘Biff’ ‘Chip’ and ‘Floppy’.  Surely teaching children about the alphabetic nature of the writing system, illustrating that there is a relationship (albeit not perfect) between the letters and sounds, is better?

In my own research, I have found no evidence that relying on a phonological reading strategy impairs children’s ability to read irregular words.  In fact, I have found the opposite.  In two studies briefly described below, I have examined a) the extent to which relying on a phonological reading strategy influences irregular word reading and b) the skills supporting children’s irregular word reading.

In the first, we found that children aged 6 -8 who took a more phonological approach to reading (i.e., relied more heavily on using phonics rules to read) performed better on assessments of irregular word reading.  In this study, irregular words were selected to be unfamiliar (i.e., low frequency), thus requiring a strategy to read them as opposed to immediate recognition.  This study was carried out with 172 children who varied in their strategy use and there was clearly a very strong relationship between dependence of a phonological reading strategy (use of phonics rules) and performance on assessments of irregular word reading (r = .66, p<.001) and standardised assessments of reading (r = .65, p<.001). 

In the second study (McGeown, Johnston & Moxon, 2014), among 180 children aged 6-9, we found that children’s nonword reading skill (ability to decode using letter-sound correspondences) was a very strong and significant predictor of their ability to read irregular words.  Indeed, it was a stronger predictor than their frequency of reading or language skills, suggesting that direct instruction in decoding skills may be appropriate to support children’s ability to read irregular words.

So why would this be the case?  Rather than categorising regular and irregular words as different word types, my collaborators and I have argued that even irregular words contain regular elements that provide a cue to pronunciation.  This is the essence of Seidenberg’s theory of quasi-regularity (Seidenberg, 2005).  For example, words such as ‘aisle’ ‘pint’ and ‘yacht’ are not completely irregular and a child with good phonics knowledge and using a phonological reading strategy will be better able to make use of the regular elements of irregular words to read.  It will not be a perfect strategy – the English writing system will not allow it; however this extra cue appears to be helpful and supportive as children face the challenging task of reading irregular words.

 
References:

 Ellis, N. C. Natsume, M., Stavropoulou, K., Hoxhallari, L., Van daal, V. H. P., Polyzoe, N., Tsipe, M-L., & Petalas, M. (2004). The effects of orthographic depth on learning to read alphabetic, syllabic, and
logographic scripts.  Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 438-468. doi:10.1598/RRQ.39.4.5.

 McGeown, S. P., Johnston,R. S., & Moxon, G. E.  (2014).  Towards an understanding of how children read and spell irregular words: the role of nonword and orthographic processing skills.  Journal of Research in Reading, 37, 51-64.  doi:10.1111/jrir.12007. 

McGeown, S. P., Medford, E., & Moxon, G. (2013).  Individual differences in children’s reading and spelling strategies and the skills supporting strategy use.  Learning and Individual Differences, 28, 75-81. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.09.013.

 Seidenberg, M.S. (2005). Connectionist models of word reading. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 238242. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00372.x
 

Friday, 1 August 2014

Non-cognitive attributes and educational outcomes


Research is accruing on something that teachers probably already knew…. that non-cognitive attributes (e.g., student confidence, motivation, personality, resilience) are significant predictors of a range of educational outcomes.  Indeed, there is an excellent (and very accessible) review of this topic within a recent Education Endowment Foundation (EEF) publication (http://educationendowmentfoundation.org.uk/uploads/pdf/Non-cognitive_skills_literature_review.pdf).

This review considers a range of non-cognitive attributes (termed ‘skills’) - namely self-perceptions, motivation, perseverance, self-control, metacognitive strategies, social competencies, resilience/coping and creativity.  The review very helpfully provides definitions of these terms, existing measurement tools to examine these attributes and correlational and causal evidence between these attributes and educational outcomes.  Furthermore, the extent to which each non-cognitive attribute is malleable (i.e., may respond to intervention) is discussed.

However, despite what seems to be increasing recognition of the importance of non-cognitive attributes, there is still, in my opinion, greater time and attention spent on studying aspects of cognition (i.e., language, memory etc) amongst psychology researchers in the UK.  This could be for a number of reasons.  From my experience as a researcher, I know that non-cognitive skills are often underestimated and are regarded (by some) as less worthy of attention than aspects of cognition.  However, they are also arguably harder to define and measure than aspects of cognition (at least at present) and this poses difficulties for conducting high quality research.  Of course, one could argue that we cannot define or measure motivation, resilience or confidence etc.  As a psychologist, I appreciate the complexity of these attributes, but I also believe that we have to develop high quality measurement tools if we want to better understand the role non-cognitive attributes play in children and young people’s lives. 

If we are genuinely interested in supporting students to achieve their potential (i.e., educational and personal success), researchers need to invest more time and effort studying non-cognitive attributes.  My research has developed in this way; from beginning my research career as someone solely interested in the cognitive skills associated with reading development, I now consider the relative importance of both cognitive and non-cognitive skills.

While understanding the relationship between non-cognitive attributes, educational and personal success is important, equally important are studies identifying home, school and community environments conducive to fostering positive attributes (e.g., motivation, perseverance), and effective interventions for those students who may be in need of support. 

However, we need more research from the UK.  While the EEF review is helpful and relatively comprehensive for teachers, it did highlight to me (from my familiarity with this field), the lack of research studies from the UK.  We need to understand the role and importance of these attributes as children proceed through the UK’s education system(s).

Reference:

Morrison Gutman, L., & Schoon, I.  (2013).  The impact of non-cognitive skills on outcomes for young people.  Education Endowment Foundation/Cabinet Office.