I am in the process of
preparing a conference paper discussing gender differences in reading (and
education more widely). However, in need
of a break from it, so thought a quick post about the topic would provide a
welcome rest….
I’ve carried out quite a
bit of research exploring gender differences in reading. While my initial publications referred to
‘gender’ differences, my preference now is to refer to ‘sex’ differences as I
am becoming increasingly interested in the distinction between the two. Indeed, the terms sex and gender are
typically used interchangeably in the research literature; however while sex
refers to biological differences between boys and girls (and is typically the
focus of research exploring sex/gender differences), gender refers to the
characteristics typically associated with being male or female. Both boys and
girls will vary in the extent to which they identify with
traditional/stereotypical masculine and feminine traits (i.e., their gender
identity). It is this variation among
boys and girls that interests me more – i.e., can sex differences commonly
found within education be better understood from a gender identity perspective?
To date, I have carried
out two studies on this topic (see references below). In Study 1 (McGeown et al., 2012) we explored
sex differences in children’s motivation
to read (girls, on average, typically report greater reading motivation) and in
Study 2 (McGeown, 2013) explored sex differences in book choices (comparing
books ‘aimed’ at boys, girls or gender neutral). In the first study, we found that children’s
(both boys and girls) identification with feminine traits was more closely
related to their motivation to read, than their identification with masculine
traits. In the second study, we found
that children’s identification with feminine traits was more closely related to
the likelihood that they would read gender neutral books (and books aimed at
girls). In addition, girls were more
likely to transcend gender boundaries when it came to book choices (i.e., were
more likely to read books marketed towards boys, than boys were to read books
marketed towards girls).
What both studies
illustrate however, is that for both boys and girls, identifying with
traditional/ stereotypical ‘feminine’ characteristics (e.g., being kind,
caring, compassionate, etc – see Boldizar 1991 for questionnaire used) is
associated with greater motivation to read and greater book reading. This suggests that reading is still perceived
as a more feminine activity.
In terms of educational significance, I have suggested
that interventions to de-feminise reading, such as providing male role models
as readers or more male orientated environments for boys to develop their
reading skills could be useful. Also, interventions that are focused towards
promoting reading between fathers and their children may also be effective at
reducing children’s early perceptions that reading is a more feminine
activity. I also suggest that careful
consideration should be given to the types of books available in schools; boys
in particular will benefit from having access to books predominately aimed at
males, as they are less likely to transcend gender boundaries and may still
perceive ‘gender neutral’ books as more feminine than masculine.
Beyond
these two studies however, I believe that this focus on gender identity could
be a helpful one for studying other ‘gender’ trends within education, as it
removes this dichotomy between boys and girls.
Indeed, any psychology/educational researcher interested in sex/gender
differences knows that there is greater within group variance (i.e., variation
among girls or variation among boys) within specific aspects of education
(e.g., attainment, motivation, etc) than there is between group variance (i.e.,
differences between boys and girls). As
noted by Hyde’s (2005) ‘gender similarities hypothesis’ an over emphasis on
studying or discussing gender differences suggests that males and females are
more different than they actually are.
Perhaps studying gender identity instead is a better way of
understanding the gender trends that are typically found within education.
References:
Boldizar, J.P. (1991). Assessing sex typing and androgyny in
children: The Children’s Sex Role Inventory. Developmental Psychology, 27(3),
505–515.
Hyde,
J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American
Psychologist, 60, 6
McGeown, S., Goodwin, H., Henderson, N., & Wright,
P. (2012). Gender differences in reading
motivation: Does sex or gender identity
provide a better account? Journal of
Research in Reading, 35(2), 328-336.
McGeown,
S. P. (2013). Sex or gender identity? Understanding children’s reading choices and
motivation. Journal of Research in
Reading. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01546.