Last week I was directed to this by Susan Godsland (thank
you Susan). Deadline is 12th
December if you’d like to submit a comment, my contribution is below (note: maximum
5000 characters):
The Education Committee invites views on the strength
of the evidence in relation to the current policy on Phonics and various
methods of learning to read. http://www.parliament.uk/business/committees/committees-a-z/commons-select/education-committee/dfe-evidence-check-forum/phonics/
My comment:
In terms of
additional research focusing on synthetic phonics instruction in UK schools, I
would like to direct the Education Committee to the following papers (see below). I believe the strength of evidence for
synthetic phonics instruction is increasing; however we need to be critical of
the evidence, impartial and ensure we rely on high quality research to inform
this debate. In response to the paper
produced by the UK Parliament, I’ve also commented on a couple of points:
The following
papers may be of interest to inform the discussion of phonics in UK schools:
Papers A and
B below examine synthetic phonics in depth and show that this is a
particularly effective method for children starting school with weak reading
readiness skills (e.g., poor letter-sound knowledge) and also for children with
weak language (i.e., vocabulary) skills. Therefore it is important to consider
individual differences (see point below) when considering the effectiveness of
phonics instruction. In addition, for schools implementing a systematic
synthetic phonics method of instruction, these papers show the skills children
are relying upon as they learn to read by this approach:
A) McGeown,
S. P., & Medford, E. (2013). Using
method of instruction to predict the skills supporting initial reading
development: insight from a synthetic phonics approach. Reading
and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 591-608.
B) McGeown,
S., Johnston, R., & Medford, E.
(2012). Reading instruction
affects the cognitive skills supporting early reading development. Learning
and Individual Differences, 22, 360-264.
Paper C compares the
long term effects of synthetic phonics vs analytic phonics instruction in UK
schools.
C) Johnston,
R. S., McGeown, S., & Watson, J. E. (2012).
Long term effects of synthetic versus analytic phonics teaching on the
reading and spelling ability of 10 year old boys and girls. Reading
and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 1365-1384.
Papers D and E deal specifically with concerns
regarding the irregular nature of the English orthography and show that use of
decoding skills/a phonological approach is actually beneficial, not
detrimental, to irregular word reading.
This is evidence against the idea that the English writing system is too
irregular for a phonics focused approach to be effective:
D) McGeown, S.
P., Johnston, R. S., & Moxon, G.
(2013). Toward an understanding
of how children read and spell irregular words: the role of nonword and orthographic
processing skills. Journal of Research in Reading,
37, 51-64.
E) McGeown,
S. P., Medford, E., & Moxon, G. (2013).
Individual differences in children’s reading and spelling strategies and
the skills supporting strategy use. Learning and Individual Differences, 28, 75-81.
Comments on points within
the UK Parliament document specifically:
“UK and international research shows
overwhelmingly that systematic phonics teaching,
in a language-rich curriculum….”.
I think it is critical that you continue to highlight that
phonics is an effective way to teach children to read new/unfamiliar words, but
is not a method to teach children the meaning of these words. I think this distinction is often confused
(i.e., phonics is criticised because children don’t learn word meanings through
this approach; however that is not the function of phonics). Therefore phonics needs to be presented to
teachers within a context; phonics is an effective method to teach children to
read new words; however children should also be exposed to literature and a
rich linguistic environment to develop their language and reading comprehension
skills (e.g., via story book activities etc).
“Sound evidence that systematic synthetic phonics programmes produce greater growth in reading than other reading programmes, and this is especially effective for younger, at-risk readers (National Reading Panel, 2000b)”
Again, I think this point is crucial. There is huge variation among children in the reading-related and cognitive skills that they start school with and a systematic synthetic phonics method of instruction is potentially most effective for children starting school knowing very few letter-sounds and with weak language skills (see Papers A and B above). However, teachers are experiencing very different student intakes and this may explain the differing views regarding the effectiveness of phonics instruction. We need to appreciate that the effectiveness of a synthetic phonics approach may differ based on student cohort. The argument that ‘one size does not fit all’ does not mean that we should be teaching children a range of strategies to read new words (e.g., former ‘searchlight’ model), but rather that we should be finding the most effective method to teach children based on their cognitive profiles (i.e., reading and language skills).